How do babies and toddlers develop inhibitory control?

Have you ever giggled at those viral videos of toddlers trying their utmost not to eat a marshmallow placed enticingly before them, having been told to wait until the grown-up returns so that they might get another? Those moments, while amusing, are actually part of a well-established psychological test known as the “marshmallow test”. Beyond the entertainment value, this test offers valuable insights into children’s self-control – more specifically, their ability to delay immediate gratification for a larger reward down the line. Researchers have linked the ability to delay gratification to various outcomes later in life, including school readiness and academic achievement (Friedman et al., 2011; Razza & Raymond, 2013).

One closely related skill to self-control is inhibitory control, a critical executive function that allows individuals to stop themselves from acting impulsively. Without inhibitory control, waiting for the marshmallow would be impossible! It is the force that prevents you from buying that coffee on your way to work when you’re trying to save money, or from typing in your old computer password after you’ve changed to a new one. Inhibitory control is pivotal across the lifespan, influencing everything from academic success (Jaekel et al., 2016) to emotional regulation (Hudson & Jacques, 2014). Much of our knowledge about inhibitory control comes from research with adults; this makes sense, there are lots of things that adults have to stop themselves from doing. And indeed, young children just aren’t very good at stopping themselves from performing a preferred or practiced response. It is often up to parents and caregivers to intervene to help them do so. We tell children when to stop, we say “no”, we teach them to “wait until it’s cooled down before we eat it, otherwise we will burn ourselves”. As such, it should not be a surprise to learn that young children have poor inhibitory control. Yet, research has shown that inhibitory control skills emerge already in the first year of life (Holmboe et al., 2008, 2018)

So how do babies and toddlers fare in terms of inhibitory control? Do they have any control skills at all? If they do, are these skills learned through experience? How do these skills broaden and develop?

During my doctoral research at the University of Oxford, I explored precisely these questions. Through a longitudinal study, we tracked infants and toddlers aged 10 months, 16 months, and 42 months to assess their inhibitory control abilities. Using a novel touchscreen task (the Early Childhood Inhibitory Touchscreen Task; Holmboe et al., 2021), we tested children’s ability to inhibit prepotent (well-learned) responses. This task involved prompting participants to touch a “happy face” button, with the majority of trials appearing on one side of the screen (prepotent trials). Touching the happy face would bring up an animation of a cartoon animal accompanied by funny sounds, whereas touching the blank button on the other side of the screen would not cause anything to happen. However, on occasion (25%; inhibitory trials), the happy face would appear on the opposite side of the screen than it usually did. This required participants to inhibit their prepotent response and make an alternate response in order to see the cartoon.

Our findings revealed that infants and toddlers exhibited limited inhibitory control, with response accuracy dropping significantly when inhibition was required (Fiske et al., 2022; 2024). However, by the age of 42 months, there was a marked improvement in inhibitory control performance (Fiske et al., in prep), suggesting significant developmental changes during this period (unsurprising when you consider the difference between a 16-month-old toddler and a 3½-year-old preschooler!). Future research should examine predictors of inhibitory control performance in the second and third years of life, as it is likely that inhibitory control abilities improve with the acquisition of language and the consolidation of motor skills involved in walking and exploring the environment in toddlerhood.

Whilst children were completing the touchscreen task, we measured their brain activity using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), a child-friendly optical imaging technique. Interestingly, even at 10 months of age, infants exhibited neural activation in the right prefrontal and parietal cortices on trials where inhibition was required – the same brain regions associated with inhibitory control in older children and adults (Fiske et al., 2022). Furthermore, the fNIRS results highlighted that, despite no improvement in inhibitory behaviour, there were changes in the neural substrates from infancy (10-months) to toddlerhood (16-months), such that activation was seen bilaterally and across more regions of the prefrontal cortex, specifically, the inferior frontal gyrus (Fiske et al., 2024). These findings could indicate that the transition to toddlerhood includes a period of neural reorganisation or specialisation in the prefrontal cortex. Finally, we found that activation in both the right inferior parietal cortex and the right inferior frontal gyrus was relatively consistent from 16-months to 3½ years, possibly marking the start of the formation of a mature response inhibition brain network (Fiske et al., in prep).

In summary, while babies and toddlers may initially struggle with inhibitory control, their abilities improve significantly with age, guided by both experience and neural development. While our understanding of inhibitory control development in infants and toddlers is still evolving, this research sheds new light on the emergence and progression of these skills across early childhood. As I continue to delve deeper into this fascinating area of research in the future, I hope to gain valuable insights into the intricate process of inhibitory control development from infancy and across the early years of life.

References

Fiske, A., de Klerk, C., Lui, K. Y., Collins-Jones, L., Hendry, A., Greenhalgh, I., … & Holmboe, K. (2022). The neural correlates of inhibitory control in 10-month-old infants: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 257, 119241.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119241

Fiske, A., Collins-Jones, L. H., de Klerk, C., Lui, K. Y., Hendry, A., Greenhalgh, I., … Holmboe, K. (2024). The Neural Correlates of Response Inhibition across the Transition from Infancy to Toddlerhood: An fNIRS study. Imaging Neuroscience, 2. 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00206

Fiske, A., Mortimer, A., Collins-Jones, L.H., de Klerk, C., Gattas, S., Dvergsdal, H., …. & Holmboe, K. (in prep). Inhibitory Control Development from Infancy to Early Childhood: A Longitudinal fNIRS Study. Pre-registered at: https://osf.io/swe2j

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., & Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: a behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental psychology, 47(5), 1410. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023750

Holmboe, K., Fearon, R. P., Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). Freeze-Frame: A new infant inhibition task and its relation to frontal cortex tasks during infancy and early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology100(2), 89-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.09.004

Holmboe, K., Bonneville‐Roussy, A., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2018). Longitudinal development of attention and inhibitory control during the first year of life. Developmental science21(6), e12690. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12690

Holmboe, K., Larkman, C., de Klerk, C., Simpson, A., Bell, M. A., Patton, L., … & Dvergsdal, H. (2021). The early childhood inhibitory touchscreen task: A new measure of response inhibition in toddlerhood and across the lifespan. PloS one, 16(12), e0260695. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260695

Hudson, A., & Jacques, S. (2014). Put on a happy face! Inhibitory control and socioemotional knowledge predict emotion regulation in 5-to 7-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123, 36-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.012

Jaekel, J., Eryigit-Madzwamuse, S., & Wolke, D. (2016). Preterm toddlers’ inhibitory control abilities predict attention regulation and academic achievement at age 8 years. The Journal of pediatrics, 169, 87-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.029

Kahle, S., Utendale, W. T., Widaman, K. F., & Hastings, P. D. (2018). Parasympathetic regulation and inhibitory control predict the development of externalizing problems in early childhood. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 46(2), 237-249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0305-6

Razza, R. A., & Raymond, K. (2013). Associations among maternal behavior, delay of gratification, and school readiness across the early childhood years. Social Development, 22(1), 180-196.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00665.x

About the Author

Abigail Fiske

Abigail Fiske

University of Oxford (UK)

Abi Fiske is a postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Experimental Psychology at the University of Oxford. Her research uses functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to probe the neural correlates of early executive function development from infancy and across early childhood.

@AbiFiske

You May Also Like…

Founding Generation Symposium 2024

Founding Generation Symposium 2024

The ICIS Founding Generation Summer Fellowship for Undergraduates aims to develop the next generation of scholars to advance innovative research on infancy and translation of research for the public good. The program pairs promising students with research mentors from...

Introducing translational parentomics

Introducing translational parentomics

Understanding and nurturing parenting as the place where development happens Vision: […] But before I go, I feel I must know. What am I? Wanda Maximoff: […] You are a body of wires and blood and bone […] You are my sadness and my hope. And mostly you're my love....

Time to shine the light on children’s rights

Time to shine the light on children’s rights

As a child development researcher, I appreciate the profound impact of childhood experiences on children’s neurodevelopmental, cognitive, social, and emotional trajectories. I, like many of my colleagues, chose this career out of a deeply rooted drive to generate...

Copyright

© 2021 by the author. Except as otherwise noted, the ICIS Baby Blog, including its text and figures, is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit:
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/4.0/legalcode

Translate »
Share This